The olfactory sense

The sense of smell, an “omission” in learning.


The attention of small children is very often solicited by calling out to them: “look” or “listen”. We also say “taste it”, but more rarely “smell it”, ignoring the fact that what we call “taste” is certainly due to the flavours perceived in the mouth, but mainly to the aromas captured by the sense of smell.

You can check this with a simple experiment. Pinch your nose before putting food in your mouth. Chew it: “It doesn’t taste good! Open your nose: “It tastes good ! Indeed, when we eat, odorants are released in the mouth and go up to the nose through the back of the throat (pharynx) to reach the olfactory epithelium where they are smelled like the odorants inspired by the nostrils; this is the retro-nasal route. But, as the two sensations coincide, they are amalgamated under the name of taste.

We attach great importance to vision and hearing, which are our dominant senses. But the loss of smell (anosmia) reveals its importance in our daily lives, as some victims of covid-19 have shown.

For the sense of smell, while it is very useful for appreciating food, also plays an implicit, but often crucial, role in human relationships, in our relationship with ourselves and with the environment.

Unfortunately, at least in the West, this sense is neglected and education leaves it untouched, leaving it to be learned at random from life’s experiences.

Eating well


However, the sense of smell functions from intra-uterine life. Between 4 and 6 months of gestation, the nose opens up, allowing the nasal circulation of amniotic fluid which brings in the mother’s odours (Schaal, 1988).

And, as the olfactory sensors[1] in the nose are functional in the last third of pregnancy, the fetus can accumulate a prenatal olfacto-gustatory experience that it will express at birth (Valentin and Chanquoy, 2012).

Researchers tested this transnatal memory by having future mothers taste aniseed. When the babies were born, they showed a preference for the smell of aniseed, whereas the babies in the control group showed indifference or aversion (Schaal et al., 2000). But for the newborn, and then for the rest of his life, the most important smell is that of his mother. It is she who guides him to the breast to suckle (Varendi et al., 1996). Then, it is thanks to breastfeeding that he will enrich his olfactory repertoire because not only does the composition of breast milk evolve very quickly according to the child’s nutritional needs, but he also carries with him the diversity of aromas from his mother’s diet (Nicklaus et al., 2005).

At the same time, these early sensory experiences usually take place in an environment that is reassuring for the baby, which favours their memorisation.

Here again, researchers have looked at the consequences of this early learning. They have found that breastfeeding, followed by diversification of the diet after 5 months, not only promotes diversity of diet at the age of 6, but also curiosity about new dishes (Maier-Nöth et al., 2016). The child is therefore better equipped both to diversify his or her diet and to enjoy it, which is good for his or her physical and mental health.

Living well together


However, this early learning, which is largely implicit and little verbalised, is not just about food. In doing so, as with the other senses, the baby also becomes imbued with the family culture and enters the circle of human olfactory communication, which is largely ignored. Yet, if a family member is offered several body odour T-shirts, children are able to recognise their own smell (Lord and Kasprzak, 1989; Perl et al., 2020), those of their siblings and those of their parents (Russell, 1976). Conversely, parents recognise a T-shirt worn by their child (Porter, 1998; Dubas et al., 2009).

Often perceived in a non-conscious way, our body odours tell us a lot about ourselves: our age, our gender, our health status, our diet. Common language tells us that we can ‘smell’ or (more often) ‘not smell’ someone. And, in couples, we see, again in the T-shirt test, that we generally prefer the smell of our partner to any other smell.

Furthermore, emotions are perceived through the nose, and they can be contagious, such as the smell of anxiety or fear (Prehn-Kristensen et al, 2009), or the smell of tears (Gelstein et al., 2011). And, among the reasons why anosmics are very unhappy is the inability to smell others, and oneself.

For personal development



Olfactory sensations are fleeting and rapidly changing. To grasp all their facets, one must exercise one’s attention, memory and curiosity. There are hardly any scientific articles on the benefits of olfactory training for small children. However, it is known that in older children, and even in adults, this exercise enables them to reach, if not an expert level such as perfumers, oenologists or cooks, at least an enlightened amateur level that enables them to describe and appreciate olfactory sensations in the same way as visual and auditory perceptions (Jehl and Murphy, 1998).

In animals where experiments have been carried out, rats or mice raised in an olfactory-rich environment (i.e. where odours change frequently) are found to improve not only their olfactory performance after a few days, but also their orientation faculties and to reduce their anxiety (which could be translated into ‘greater self-confidence’; Veyrac et al., 2009).

This training therefore facilitates concentration, introspection and memory (Coffield et al., 2014).

A simple olfactory awakening offered in a nursery (not an apprenticeship, just showing toddlers that they have a nose and that they can use it) reveals that from the very first workshops, children become curious to exercise their sense of smell and taste, thus preparing them to take in all the dimensions of their multisensory environment. It is also known that, under certain circumstances, odours promote learning (Rasch et al., 2007).

Given the paucity of studies already published, it is not yet clear what benefits they may derive, but, for example, it is known that, in general, people with a good sense of smell are more sociable and more likely to seek compromise, perhaps because they ‘smell’ people better (Mori et al., 2015).

The world our children enter is already very fragrant, although we do not always pay attention to it. In fact, isn’t everything in our daily lives scented? Hygiene and household products, means of transport, shops, hotels, etc. To maintain self-control in these environments, you need to be educated.

Aromachology (or olfactotherapy), a gentle practice that suggests scents that can bring well-being, can also be used (Angelucci et al., 2014).

We are also seeing the development of olfactory medical diagnosis: from odours analysed in the breath, we can now diagnose diseases with an astonishing success rate of around 85-90%. These devices will become part of our health environment in the coming years (Salesse, 2018).

Finally, the covid epidemic has also reminded us that culture is an indispensable dimension of our existence. Olfactory art, beyond perfumery, also brings an additional sensory dimension to our aesthetic emotions (Le Guérer , 2002; Jaquet, 2015).
impact of learning: at the age of 15 years old, breastfed children like vegetables a little more.
Angelucci F.L., Silva V.V., Dal Pizzol C., Spir L.G., Praes C.E., and Maibach H. (2014) Physiological effect of olfactory stimuli inhalation in humans: an overview. International Journal of Cosmetic Science, 36:2, 117-123.

Buck L.B and Axel R. (1991) A novel multigene family may encode odorant receptors: a molecular basis for odor recognition. Cell, 65:1, 175-187.

Coffield C.N., Mayhew E.M., Haviland-Jones J.M., Walker-Andrews A.S. (2014) Adding odor: Less distress and enhanced attention for 6-month-olds. Infant Behavior and Development, 37:2, 155-161.

Dubas J.S., Heijkoop M. and van Aken M.A.G. (2009) A preliminary investigation of parent-progeny olfactory recognition and parental investment. Human Nature, 20:1, 80-92.

Gelstein S., Yeshurun Y., Rozenkrantz L., Shushan S., Frumin I., Roth Y. and Sobel N. (2011) Human tears contain a chemosignal. Science, 331:6014, 226-230.

Jaquet C. (under the direction of, 2015) L’art olfactif contemporain. Classiques Garnier, Paris, 317 p.

Jehl C. and Murphy C. (1998) Development effects on odor learning and memory in children. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 855:1, 632-634.

Le Guérer A. (2002) Les pouvoirs de l’odeur. Ed. Odile Jacob, Paris, 320 p.

Lord T. and Kasprzak M. (1989) Identification of self through olfaction. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 69:1: 219-224.

Russell, M.J. (20176) Human olfactory communication. Nature, 260:5551, 520-522.

Maier-Nöth A., Schaal B., Leathwood P. and Issanchou S. (2016) The lasting influences of early food-related variety experience: A longitudinal study of vegetable acceptance from 5 months to 6 years in two populations. Plos One, 11:3, e 0151356.

Mori E., Petters W., Schriever V.A., Valder C. and Hummel T. (2015) Exposure to odours improves olfactory functions in healthy children. Rhinology, 53:3, 221-226.

Nicklaus S., Boggio V. and Issanchou S. (2005) Taste perceptions in children. Archives de Pédiatrie, 12:5, 57-584.

Prehn-Kristensen A., Wiesner C., Bergmann T.O., Wolff S., Jansen O., Mehdorn H.M., Ferstl,R., Pause B.M. (2009) Induction of empathy by the smelle of anxiety. Pols One, 4:6, e5987.

Perl O., Mishor E., Ravia A., Ravreby I. and Sobel N. (2020) Are humans constantly but subconsciously smelling themselves? Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society, B:Biological Sciences, 375:1800, 20190372.

Porter R.H. (1998) Olfaction and human kin recognition. Genetica, 104:3, 259-263.

Rasch B., Büchel C., Gais S. and Born J. (2007) Odor cues during slow-wave sleep prompt declarative memory consolidation. Science, 315:5817, 1426-1429.

Salesse R. (2018) Electronic nose: the future of medical diagnostics? Sciences Psy, n° 13, p 66-69.

Schaal B. (1988) Olfaction in infants and children: developmental and functional perspectives. Chemical Senses, 13:2, 145-190.

Schaal B., Marlier L. and Soussignan R. (2000) Human foetuses learn odours from their pregnant mother’s diet. Chemical Senses, 25:6, 729-737.

Valentin D and Chanquoy L. (2012) Olfactory categorization: a developmental study. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 113:3, 337-352.

Varendi H., Porter R.H. and Winberg J. (1996) Attractiveness of amniotic fluid odor: evidence of prenatal olfactory learning? Acta Paediatrica, 85, 1223-1227.

Veyrac A., Sacquet J., Nguyen V., Marien M., Jourdan F., and Didier A. (2009) Novelty determines the effects of olfactory enrichment on memory and neurogenesis through noradrenergic mechanisms. Neuropsychopharmacology, 34:3, 786-795.